Blog

Moral Intuitions on Food

I’m about half-way through Jonathan Haidt’s new book, The Righteous Mind.  In the book, he makes the case that our moral judgments are mainly based on intuitive reactions.  We only make up logical reasons for our judgments later (if we can) to justify our initial intuitions.  Bailey Norwood and I made a similar case in terms of how we think about the rightness or wrongness of caging farm animals in chapter 6 of our recent book, Compassion by the Pound.  

What struck me as I read Haidt was his discussion on moral disagreement.  It is very had to change someone’s intuitions about what is right or wrong.  If we can’t even articulate the reasons why we think something is wrong, how can someone possibly make a compelling, reasoned counter-argument?  Haidt argues that trying to use reason to change someone’s moral intuition is a bit like trying to make a dog happy by grabbing its tail and wagging it. 

So, how is it that I intuitively feel so differently about various aspects of food production (e.g., biotechnology, irradiation, pesticides, herbicides, etc.) than others who are revolted by the same issues?  When I think about these issues, I am not appalled; I don’t feel any disgust.  But, I suspect I’m in the minority of Americans. 

I gave the Shepard lecture last night to a group of students and faculty at Kenyon College about the future of food.  Although we had a civil, productive discussion, it’s safe to say that many of the students in the room had different moral intuitions about these topics and I do.  Their moral intuitions are that many modern food technologies are self-evidently wrong (while other issues like local, organic, and natural are self-evidently right). 

How is it that our moral intuitions can be so different?  I grew up around “big ag.”  I’ve personally sprayed Monsanto’s Round-Up on hundreds of acres of cotton weeds.  I’ve personal castrated farm animals to limit aggression and off-tasting meat.  I’ve personally had to throw away thousands of pounds of salsa that grew mold because adequate levels of preservatives weren't added.  I’ve personally met and know people who work for Monsanto, Cargill, ADM, etc.  I grew up going to school with kids whose parents were immigrant farm laborers living at or below poverty. 

Now, that doesn’t necessarily make my intuitions about modern food production somehow objectively correct.  But, I at least can lay claim to the fact that they are based on actual life experiences and insights. 

That said, I suspect there were more than a few pre-civil war southerners whose life experiences led them to believe slavery was o.k.  On the flip side, there are many examples of people having faulty (at least what many of us would now say are faulty) moral intuitions on topics for which they had very little experience (e.g., the wrongness of eating pork).  Actual life experience with the issue in question may or may not correlate well with faulty moral intuitions.

I don’t know exactly where that leaves us except to say that Haidt argues that moral persuasion tends to work more on the social level than the cognitive.  According to Haidt, If you think I’m a nice guy, you’re more likely to give my moral intuitions a test-drive. 

Here’s hoping that, despite the facts and logical arguments given in my talk last night, I came across as a nice guy.

Whose Afraid of GMOs?

We humans are notoriously bad at judging relative risks.  We worry about some things that are very unlikely to happen (e.g., getting bit by a shark while visiting the beach), while ignoring other activities that are much riskier (e.g., driving to the beach).     ​

​This economic meme by Art Carden humorously points out such an inconsistency in the way we think about many food risks

econmicmeme.jpg

Do Your Friends Know You Better Than You Know Yourself?

According to Science News, ​new findings from a longitudinal study following kids since 1976 revealed some surprising results.  Here is the basic study set up according to the authors

Over two years, Montreal students in grades 1, 4 and 7 completed peer evaluations of their classmates and rated them in terms of aggression, likeability and social withdrawal. The students also did self-evaluations

The outcome?​

We found the evaluations from the group of peers were much more closely associated with eventual adult outcomes than were their own personality perceptions from childhood.

​These findings tie nicely in with the research Bailey Norwood and I have conducted over the past several years (see here, here, or here), where we argue (and find) that the best way to predict what someone will do  in the grocery store is often not to ask them what they will do but rather to ask them what they think someone else will do.  

The human mind is skilled in the art of deception (including self deception).  This research reminds me of a quote by Richard Feynman:​

The first principle is that you must not fool yourself—and you are the easiest person to fool.